Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance

Papers
(The TQCC of Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance is 3. The table below lists those papers that are above that threshold based on CrossRef citation counts [max. 250 papers]. The publications cover those that have been published in the past four years, i.e., from 2020-05-01 to 2024-05-01.)
ArticleCitations
Salience determines attentional orienting in visual selection.40
Attentional suppression of highly salient color singletons.29
The eye–mind wandering link: Identifying gaze indices of mind wandering across tasks.25
Proactive enhancement and suppression elicited by statistical regularities in visual search.19
Statistical regularities across trials bias attentional selection.16
Examining bilingual language switching across the lifespan in cued and voluntary switching contexts.15
Learned distractor rejection in the face of strong target guidance.14
Spatial alignment facilitates visual comparison.14
Task sets serve as boundaries for the congruency sequence effect.14
Attentional control and metacognitive monitoring of the effects of different types of task-irrelevant sound on serial recall.13
Overt attentional capture by reward-related stimuli overcomes inhibitory suppression.13
Individual representations in visual working memory inherit ensemble properties.11
Timing is everything: Onset timing moderates the crossmodal influence of background sound on taste perception.11
Affordance matching predictively shapes the perceptual representation of others’ ongoing actions.11
Fixation, flexibility, and creativity: The dynamics of mind wandering.11
A critical analysis of the functional parameters of the quiet eye using immersive virtual reality.10
Probabilistic cuing of visual search: Neither implicit nor inflexible.10
Guidance of attention by working memory is a matter of representational fidelity.10
Twofold advantages of face processing with or without visual awareness.10
Gradient activation of speech categories facilitates listeners’ recovery from lexical garden paths, but not perception of speech-in-noise.10
Refixation patterns of mind-wandering during real-world scene perception.10
Introspective awareness of oculomotor attentional capture.10
The supernumerary rubber hand illusion revisited: Perceived duplication of limbs and visuotactile events.10
Affective priming enhances gaze cueing effect.10
Attentional capture by context cues, not inhibition of cue singletons, explains same location costs.9
Examining the effects of goal-setting, feedback, and incentives on sustained attention.9
Response, rather than target detection, triggers the attentional boost effect in visual search.9
Social attention triggered by eye gaze and walking direction is resistant to temporal decay.9
Statistical learning affects the time courses of salience-driven and goal-driven selection.9
Individual differences in lapses of sustained attention: Ocolumetric indicators of intrinsic alertness.9
Target–background segregation in a spatial interference paradigm reveals shared and specific attentional mechanisms triggered by gaze and arrows.9
Motor representations evoked by objects under varying action intentions.9
Response–response bindings do not decay for 6 seconds after integration: A case for bindings’ relevance in hierarchical action control.8
Concurrent working memory load may increase or reduce cognitive interference depending on the attentional set.8
The influence of perceptual–motor variability on the perception of action boundaries for reaching.8
Gaze elicits social and nonsocial attentional orienting: An interplay of shared and unique conflict processing mechanisms.8
Representing action in terms of what not to do: Evidence for inhibitory coding during multiple action control.8
Visual illusions as a tool to hijack numerical perception: Disentangling nonsymbolic number from its continuous visual properties.8
Dynamic inhibitory control prevents salience-driven capture of visual attention.8
Speaking with an alien voice: Flexible sense of agency during vocal production.7
The role of location in the organization of bindings within short-term episodic traces.7
Sex differences in tests of mental rotation: Direct manipulation of strategies with eye-tracking.7
Multitasking strategies make the difference: Separating processing-code resources boosts multitasking efficiency when individuals prefer to interleave tasks in free concurrent dual tasking.7
Online sensory feedback during active search improves tactile localization.7
The eye wants what the heart wants: Female face preferences are related to partner personality preferences.7
Direct evidence for the optimal tuning of attention.7
You cannot “count” how many items people remember in visual working memory: The importance of signal detection–based measures for understanding change detection performance.7
Ignoring the unknown: Attentional suppression of unpredictable visual distraction.7
What can be learned in a context-specific proportion congruence paradigm? Implications for reproducibility.7
Auditory perceptual learning depends on temporal regularity and certainty.7
Snarcing with a phone: The role of order in spatial-numerical associations is revealed by context and task demands.7
Confidence can be automatically integrated across two visual decisions.6
Attentional suppression in time and space.6
A lingering question addressed: Reading rate and most efficient listening rate are highly similar.6
Perceptual competition between targets and distractors determines working memory access and produces intrusion errors in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) tasks.6
Social relevance modulates multisensory integration.6
The time course of categorical and perceptual similarity effects in visual search.6
Individual differences in perception of the speech-to-song illusion are linked to musical aptitude but not musical training.6
Unseeing the white bear: Negative search criteria guide visual attention through top-down suppression.6
On the organization of task-order and task-specific information in dual-task situations.6
Causal evidence for dissociable roles of the prefrontal and superior medial frontal cortices in decision strategies.6
Combining the senses: The role of experience- and task-dependent mechanisms in the development of audiovisual simultaneity perception.6
The role of temporal order of relevant and irrelevant dimensions within conflict tasks.6
Past on the ground floor and future in the attic: The vertical mental timeline.6
Effects of various executive functions on adults’ and children’s walking.6
The power of the self: Anchoring information processing across contexts.6
Spatial cueing effects are not what we thought: On the timing of attentional deployment.6
Statistical learning of across-trial regularities during serial search.6
The outlier paradox: The role of iterative ensemble coding in discounting outliers.6
Assessing the robustness of feature-based selection in visual working memory.6
Do effects of visual contrast and font difficulty on readers’ eye movements interact with effects of word frequency or predictability?6
It makes sense, so I see it better! Contextual information about the visual environment increases its perceived sharpness.6
An SEM approach to validating the psychological model of musical groove.5
Expectations affect the contribution of tonic global inhibition, but not of phasic global inhibition to motor imagery.5
Liking of art and the perception of color.5
Exploring task switch costs in a color-shape decision task via a mouse tracking paradigm.5
Examining mechanistic explanations for ideomotor effects.5
Quantitative and qualitative differences in the top-down guiding attributes of visual search.5
Executing the homebound path is a major source of error in homing by path integration.5
One-handed motor activity does not interfere with naming lateralized pictures of tools.5
Establishing the separable contributions of spatial attention and saccade preparation across tasks with varying acuity demands.5
Attentional priority is determined by predicted feature distributions.5
Only time will tell the future: Anticipatory saccades reveal the temporal dynamics of time-based location and task expectancy.5
Is working memory inherently more “precise” than long-term memory? Extremely high fidelity visual long-term memories for frequently encountered objects.5
Investigating how the modularity of visuospatial attention shapes conscious perception using type I and type II signal detection theory.5
Eye-tracking the time course of distal and global speech rate effects.5
Causality shifts the perceived temporal order of audiovisual events.5
Sequence learning is surprisingly fragile in visual search.5
Which can explain the pip-and-pop effect during a visual search: Multisensory integration or the oddball effect?5
Towards the boundaries of self-prioritization: Associating the self with asymmetric shapes disrupts the self-prioritization effect.5
Prioritization in visual attention does not work the way you think it does.5
Subjective confidence acts as an internal cost-benefit factor when choosing between tasks.5
Task sets define boundaries of learned cognitive flexibility in list-wide proportion switch manipulations.5
Does it help to expect distraction? Attentional capture is attenuated by high distractor frequency but not by trial-to-trial predictability.5
Visual and postural eye-height information is flexibly coupled in the perception of virtual environments.4
Stroking trajectory shapes velocity effects on pleasantness and other touch percepts.4
What makes a scene? Fast scene categorization as a function of global scene information at different resolutions.4
Visual continuity during blinks and alterations in time perception.4
When a smile is a conflict: Affective mismatch between facial displays and group membership induces conflict and triggers cognitive control.4
No evidence that self-rated negative emotion boosts visual working memory precision.4
Appealing to the cognitive miser: Using demand avoidance to modulate cognitive flexibility in cued and voluntary task switching.4
Conflict-monitoring theory in overtime: Is temporal learning a viable explanation for the congruency sequence effect?4
Thought dynamics under task demands: Evaluating the influence of task difficulty on unconstrained thought.4
New templates interfere with existing templates depending on their respective priority in visual working memory.4
Localizing modality compatibility effects: Evidence from dual-task interference.4
Predicting phonology in language comprehension: Evidence from the visual world eye-tracking task in Mandarin Chinese.4
Intentional binding: Merely a procedural confound?4
There is more to contextual cuing than meets the eye: Improving visual search without attentional guidance toward predictable target locations.4
The visual system does not compute a single mean but summarizes a distribution.4
How feature context alters attentional template switching.4
On the automaticity of reactive item-specific control as evidenced by its efficiency under load.4
Are you looking at me? Impact of eye contact on object-based attention.4
When instructions don't help: Knowing the optimal strategy facilitates rule-based but not information-integration category learning.4
Out of sight, out of mind: Foveal processing is necessary for semantic integration of words into sentence context.4
Selective suppression of taboo information in visual word recognition: Evidence for cognitive control on semantics.4
Me first? Positioning self in the attentional hierarchy.4
How is location defined? Implications for learning and transfer of location-specific control.4
Do individual differences in face recognition ability moderate the other ethnicity effect?4
Interindividual differences influence multisensory processing during spatial navigation.4
Evidence of weight-based representations of gravitational motion.4
What triggers a gesture? Exploring affordance compatibility effects in representational gesture production.3
Persistent guidance of attention in visual statistical learning.3
Phonetic category activation predicts the direction and magnitude of perceptual adaptation to accented speech.3
Selective attention operates on the group level for interactive biological motion.3
The role of objective and introspective switch costs in voluntary task choice.3
Distractor–distractor interactions in visual search for oriented targets explain the increased difficulty observed in nonlinearly separable conditions.3
To respond or not to respond? A model-based comparison between the processing of go, nogo, and neutral stimuli.3
May the force be against you: Better visual sensitivity to speed changes opposite to gravity.3
Divergent response-time patterns in vigilance decrement tasks.3
Task format modulates the relationship between reading ability and Stroop interference.3
Simple shapes guide visual attention based on their global outline or global orientation contingent on search goals.3
Association between action kinematics and emotion perception across adolescence.3
More efficient shielding for internal than external attention? Evidence from asymmetrical switch costs.3
The interplay of long-term memory and working memory: When does object-color prior knowledge affect color visual working memory?3
Perception of higher-order affordances for kicking in soccer.3
The role of attention in anticipated action effects.3
Visual working memory impairs visual detection: A function of working memory load or sensory load?3
Social attention as a general mechanism? Demonstrating the influence of stimulus content factors on social attentional biasing.3
Is zjudge a better prime for JUDGE than zudge is?: A new evaluation of current orthographic coding models.3
The transfer of global and local processing modes.3
Evidence for initially independent monitoring of responses and response effects.3
Predictive extrapolation of observed body movements is tuned by knowledge of the body biomechanics.3
Multimodal sensory integration: Diminishing returns in rhythmic synchronization.3
Decomposing the attentional blink.3
Oculometric indicators of individual differences in preparatory control during the antisaccade task.3
The rise and fall of face recognition awareness across the life span.3
Learned spatial suppression is not always proactive.3
The causal role of vision in the development of spatial coordinates: Evidence from visually impaired children.3
Visuoproprioceptive conflict in hand position biases tactile localization on the hand surface.3
Perceptual constancy with a novel sensory skill.3
Preparing for simultaneous action and inaction: Temporal dynamics and target levels of inhibitory control.3
Tones disrupt visual fixations and responding on a visual-spatial task.3
Integration and segmentation conflict during ensemble coding of shape.3
Guidance of visual search by negative attentional templates depends on task demands.3
Separating the effects of visual working memory load and attentional zoom on selective attention.3
Tuning the ensemble: Incidental skewing of the perceptual average through memory-driven selection.3
The best fitting of three contemporary observer models reveals how participants’ strategy influences the window of subjective synchrony.3
Movement drift in optic ataxia reveals deficits in hand state estimation in oculocentric coordinates.3
Supplemental Material for Interindividual Differences Influence Multisensory Processing During Spatial Navigation3
Learning to suppress likely distractor locations in visual search is driven by the local distractor frequency.3
Kinaesthetic cues when predicting the outcomes of the actions of others.3
Selective adaptation in speech: Measuring the effects of visual and lexical contexts.3
The tradeoff between item and order information in short-term memory does not depend on encoding time.3
Set size effects in spatial updating are independent of the online/offline updating strategy.3
0.037770986557007